“I love the freedoms here, but I still miss home”: Muslim Women’s Perceptions of How Social Contact Optimized Wellbeing and Personal Commitments to Faith
Skip other details (including permanent urls, DOI, citation information)
:
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 3.0 License. Please contact : [email protected] to use this work in a way not covered by the license.
For more information, read Michigan Publishing's access and usage policy.
Author Note
This research was supported in part by the Research Experience for Undergraduates program funded through the National Science Foundation. There are no conflicts of interest from authors toward the production of this manuscript.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Andrew S. Walters, Department of Psychological Sciences, Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ. Email: [email protected]
Abstract
A mixed methods (quantitative and qualitative) design was used to explore the experiences of Muslim women moving to the United States from countries where either Islam is a dominant religion or Muslims comprise a majority of the population. Participants completed established scales measuring both psychosocial adjustment and various dimensions of spirituality. In addition, participants completed a biographic interview which assessed women’s sentiments about moving to the United States, their perceptions of treatment in traveling to or once arrived in the U.S., and how newly formed relationships were perceived to have affected participants’ intrapersonal, social, and spiritual experience. The contact hypothesis (Allport, 1954) guided the development of biographic interview questions. Participants scored similarly to population-based samples on indices of psychosocial adjustment and they reported high scores on measures assessing spiritual beliefs, strength of faith, and the Reflective Commitment subscale of the Islamic Reflections Scale. Three distinct themes emerged from interpretive analyses following in-depth interviews: 1) participants were aware of media-projected antipathy toward Muslims emerging from inflammatory rhetoric ensconced by political posturing; 2) the reception women received from the majority of American neighbors, friends, and coworkers was very positive; and 3) relationships formed with other Muslims – importantly, persons who also immigrated from other Muslim-majority countries – provided opportunities for personal reflection on the differences across cultural interpretations and practices of faith.
Keywords: Contact hypothesis, Islamic reflection, Muslim-identifying women, Qualitative-quantitative mixed design, Reflective Commitment, Spirituality
It is estimated that approximately 3.45 million Muslims (or 1.1% of the population) live in the United States (Mohamed, 2018). Muslim migration to economic powerhouse countries such as Germany and the United States began visibly increasing during the 1960s (a result, in part, of reforms to some immigration statutes; Salem, 2013), but the rate of population growth has risen in the past decade, such that, at present, estimates are that approximately 100,000 new persons identifying as Muslim are added to the U.S. population each year (Mohamed, 2018). A substantive turning point in the academic scholarship specifically including the lived experiences of Muslims and of Islamic traditions within the United States can be attributed to the sequelae of events occurring in the U.S. on September 11, 2001 (Abu-Raiya, Pargament, & Mahoney, 2011; Khan & Ecklund, 2012; O’Connor & Jahan, 2014; Shammas, 2009; Sheridan & Gillett, 2005). Amer and Bagasra (2013) document a steep slope of published research in the ten years following September 11, 2001.
Considerable research articulates the breadth of individual, social, and faith-based stressors Muslims experienced after 9/11 (e.g., Abu-Raiya et al., 2011; Abu-Ras & Suárez, 2009; Gaddis & Ghoshal, 2015; Jasperse, Ward, & Jose, 2012; Jung, 2012; Khan & Ecklund, 2012; O’Connor & Jahan, 2014; Martin, 2015; Nadal, Griffin, Hamit, Leon, Tobio, & Rivera, 2012; Sheridan, 2006). American Muslims report varying experiences of perceived discrimination, including microaggressions (Ghumman & Ryan, 2013; Hauslohner; 2017; Nadal et al., 2012; O’Connor & Jahan, 2014; Martin, 2015; Moradi & Hasan, 2004; Sheridan & Gillett, 2005; Widner & Chicoine, 2011) and a widespread, palpable experience of socially sanctioned negative attitudes (Cimino, 2005; Ghaffari & Ciftci, 2010). For at least some, perceptions of others’ mistrust, surveillance, or prejudice intersects with race (Everett et al., 2015; Halliday, 1999), gender (Davids, 2014; Hartigan, 2006/2007; Mir, 2011), and the integration of faith into identity and religious practice. For example, a large, interdisciplinary body of scholarship focuses on the intersection of identities and geography in relation to Muslim women choices regarding hijab (e.g., see Furseth, 2011; Hu, Pazaki, Al-Qubbaj, & Cutler, 2009; Mishra & Shirazi, 2010; Mouhktar & Walters, 2015; William & Vashi, 2007; Zimmerman, 2014). Religious orientation and religious identity have been found to be associated with intra-psychological processes, cognitive and emotional approaches to coping, health outcomes, and cross-faith values, such as empathy and compassion (Amed & Ezzeddine, 2009; Fisher, Ai, Aydin, Frey, & Haslam, 2010; Goldfried & Miner, 2002; Khan, Watson, & Habib, 2005; Oveis, Horberg, & Keltner, 2010; Samari, 2016).
At a national level, the dominant discourses about Muslims – both those in the United States and elsewhere – and of Islam became increasingly derogatory when, in 2015, a candidate for the United States presidency contaminated public perceptions of Muslims with actively untruthful claims. Stoking fears of otherness in a segment of voters unconcerned with displays of prejudice toward non-majority persons, averse to intergroup associations with others unlike themselves (Pettigrew, 2017), and steeped with inflections of fear and nationalism (Chacón, 2017), this candidate emboldened what may have been previously unrevealed prejudices with claims such as “Islam hates us” (Hjelmgaard, 2018). This same candidate, commanding nationally targeted television air time, claimed to have witnessed “thousands and thousands of people [Muslims] were cheering” at the collapse of the Word Trade Center Towers. It is important to note that there are no data to support this claim – that is, there are no documented witnesses or video footage – and, despite evidence to the contrary (that is, that the candidate appeared to have simply fabricated a story to an Alabama audience), these hollow claims were replicated and adopted by segments of the voting population (Carroll, 2015; Nuzzi, 2017; Sides, Tesler, & Vavreck, 2017; Waldman, 2018). The fervor to support and eventually elect a candidate whose central messages were based on creating uncertainty and fear toward persons with whom potential voters had no contact, who were postured as wholly different from “true” Americans (Major, Blodorn, & Blascovich, 2018), and who threatened the (non-stated but palpably salient) deservedness of white privilege (Giroux, 2017; Huber, 2016) granted permission among some segments of society to display emboldened bigoted behavior. Even in the recent past there have been negative consequences for Muslims as a result of the current political climate (Hauslohner, 2017; Pirani, 2018); nonetheless, Muslim Americans continue to see a path forward for themselves and their families in the United States (Pew Research Center, 2017).
These national and political events – the long-standing sequelae of 9/11 and a duplicitous but incoherent political culture – potentiate mental and behavioral health risks for Muslim Americans and Muslims non-citizens who live in the United States. Adjusting to new cultural, geographic, and educational (or employment) expectations and roles may strain psychological and emotional resources. Although the increase of Muslims in the United States is in part based on births to Muslim-identifying parents and to religious conversion, the majority of the population rate increase is attributed to migration. Thus, the stresses associated with relocating, often speaking a second (or third) language, and establishing a social support network can tax or exceed a person’s cognitive and emotional resources, thus elevating a risk of maladjustment (Adam & Ward, 2016; Goforth, Oka, Leong, & Denis, 2014; Stuart, 2014; Hassouneh & Kulwicki, 2007; also see Moffic, Peteet, Zakaria Hankir, & Awaad, 2019, for a comprehensive anthology of clinical implication of working with Muslim patients and clients). A legacy of faith-based assumptions about needing or requesting emotional or psychological help – a culturally grounded origination of mental health stigma – can also be salient to persons experiencing poor(er) mental health outcomes (Amri & Bemak, 2013; Ciftci, Jones, & Corrigan, 2013; Phillips & Lauterbach, 2017). Although the nature of Islamophobia can be conceptualized macro-sociologically (Marranci, 2004; Taras, 2012), the sequelae of experiencing individual- or institutional-based Islamophobia permeates mental and physical wellbeing (Amer & Bagasra, 2013; Samari, 2016; Samari, Alcalá, & Sharif, 2018; Yilmaz, 2016).
Although psychological resources such as self-esteem, perceptions of self-efficacy, and religiosity can buffer negative experiences (e.g., such as expressed prejudice, discriminatory treatment), these intrapersonal resources can go only so far in maintaining indices of adjustment (Ghaffari & Ciftci, 2010; Jasperse et al., 2012). It may be difficult for some persons to disentangle the cause of microaggressions, overt hostility, or active discrimination because many Muslims in the U.S. are both immigrants from non-European countries and (often) are non-white; thus, attributing an isolated cause of perceived maltreatment (e.g., “It must be because I am Muslim.”) may seem futile or at least ambiguous. In these ways, it is reasonable to speculate that at least some Muslim experience what has been coined racial battle fatigue; although much of the research in the arena of racial battle fatigue has focused on persons identifying as black/African American or Latino (Franklin, Smith, & Hung, 2014; Smith, Mustaffa, Jones, Curry, & Allen, 2016; Smith, Yosso, & Solórzano, 2006; Yosso, Smith, Ceja, & Solórzano, 2009), certainly foundational premises that characterize this set of stressors is relevant to persons of color immigrating to the United States (Blackwood, Hopkins, & Reicher, 2015; Cahill, 2010). Apart from identifying as Muslim, for many persons, the perception of racism seems reasonably plausible in attributing perceived negative treatment from others (Ciftci et al., 2013; Phillips & Lauterbach, 2017).
There is a long-standing history across professional disciplines that interrogate the cognitive processes and multiples biases that manifest themselves in prejudices toward others. Although the assumption that persons living in urban environments – where persons of different historical and cultural legacies often reside in greater numbers – display less overt prejudice toward social outgroups often is substantiated (Côté & Erickson, 2009), biases, once formed, often are difficult to change irrespective of geographic location. Persons who identify as a member of a minority group (religion, race, or immigrant status are most often studied) are themselves susceptible both to the same processes that create biases (Islam & Hewstone, 1993; Rowatt, Franklin, & Cotton, 2005) and can use compensatory mechanisms that minimize the perception of discrimination to maintain or improve positive self-schema (Ruggiero & Taylor, 1997). Results from experimental studies both expand an understanding for how biases might be increased or minimized (Everett et al., 2015) and provide opportunities for how positive attitudes and positive behavioral intentions among persons across social groups can be increased (Hutchison & Rosenthal, 2011).
At the individual level, there may be mechanisms or strategies for persons to buffer or minimize the negative effects of larger-scale prejudice – including both perceived and actualized prejudice – but these resources are variable across individuals, often finite, and can be especially taxed when a group is designated, usually arbitrarily, as a social outgroup. Over its history, documented tensions based on religion have characterized in-group/out-group relationships in the United States; nuances to this discourse might include, for example, differences between Catholics and Protestants or mainline Christian churches compared to fundamentalist and evangelical denominations. Religiously articulated biases, and social effects of those biases onto designated outgroups, is certainly not new. In the more current era, sentiments about Islam and of Muslims (or even perhaps individuals who might be perceived as Muslim) constitute a (at least politically) sanctioned outgroup. The individual and summative effects of biases against, and differential treatment of, persons identifying as Muslim are often collectively referred to as Islamophobia.
Although definitions of Islamophobia might vary, they generally coalesce around what Bleich (2011) articulates as the “indiscriminate negative attitudes or emotions directed at Islam or Muslims (p. 1593). The Islamophobia concept has been interrogated by researchers across a number of disciplines. Choma, Haji, Hodson, and Hoffarth (2016) reported, from three small samples of Christian-identifying college students, that “intergroup disgust sensitivity” – that is, persons’ dispositional responses of disgust or revulsion to outgroups – “robustly” predicted Islamophobia (p. 50), while Lopes and Jaspal (2015) found across two experiments that anti-Muslim prejudice was predicted by trait paranoia. Persons who score higher on measures of Islamophobia have been found to endorse negative stereotypes of Muslims (Sides & Gross, 2013) and to expect Muslims to forfeit their native culture (Kunst, Sadeghi, Tahir, Sam, & Thomsen, 2016). Lajevardi and Oskooii (2018) have shown that the more current, derogatory prejudices against Muslims show a parallel foundation to what they call “old-fashioned racism” (p.114); over time and circumstance, outgroups may change but the undercurrent processes in establishing ingroups and outgroups operate similarly. And while Merino (2010) found that those Americans who believe the United States is a Christian nation extend less attitudinal support for the inclusion of Hindus and Muslims into community life, he also found that interpersonal contact with Buddhists, Hindus, and Muslims – as might be supported by the contact hypothesis – is related to favorable attitudes in relation to religious diversity and an increased tolerance for the inclusion of a mosque in one’s local community. Thus, although components of both the enactment of Islamophobia and in experiencing the effects of Islamophobia may share similarities with other socially or culturally othered outgroups, easy access to some television programming or online platforms incontrovertibly document the presence of Islamophobia in multiple spheres of contemporary society in the United States and other industrialized countries.
The contact hypothesis is an enormously rich theoretical lens through which to conceptualize and investigate bias, prejudice, and discrimination. Historically, the contact hypothesis emerged from a chapter of Allport’s paradigm-shifting classic work The Nature of Prejudice (1954). This theoretical framework has been used by researchers, interventionists, educators, and practitioners, has been the basis of literally hundreds[1] of empirical studies, and is still seen as a solid theoretical tool nearly 70 years after its inception (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2005). The original version of the contact hypothesis speculated that the probability for positive outcomes (such as increased favorable attitudes toward or less prejudice about minorities) increased if members of one group (often referred to as an ingroup) had contact with the minority group (often referred to as the outgroup). Positive outcomes are often believed to be maximized if a number of conditions are met (i.e., shared goals, contact occurs in a context where members have equal status). Early review articles consistently found that, when the favorable conditions were met – in research, in practice, in applied contexts such as neighborhoods or persons from different countries (even countries in conflict with one another) – reductions in prejudice in fact occurred (Amir, 1969).
Early research guided by the contact hypothesis was focused on race relations (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2005). Over time, and with literally hundreds of studies completed, the contact hypothesis has been a foundational lens to study dozens of ingroup/outgroup relations. Research study methodologies have included variations of ethnography, in-person interventions using qualitative methods such as focus groups, and experimental designs. Experimental studies document the powerful influences of in-group bias across conditions (Ben-Ner, McCall, Stephane, & Wang, 2009) but also show that disclosure – what stems from emerging relationships with outgroup members – reduces ingroup bias (Ensari & Miller, 2002). More recent research has shown that secondary or even “imagined intergroup contact” results in positive outcomes (Crisp & Turner, 2009, p. 231; Pettigrew, 2009) and that “mundane” activities with others (different from self) should be seen as agents of personal attitude and prejudice reduction (Dixon, Durrheim, & Tredoux, 2005, p. 707). At the broadest conceptual level, primary goals of the contact hypothesis are to move persons beyond a “Us - Them” scheme to a “We” scheme (Gaertner, Dovidio, & Bachman, 1996), to understand the individual processes that facilitate – or resist – change, to maximize intergroup relations (e.g., see Hutchison & Rosenthal, 2011; Henry & Hardin, 2006; Pettigrew, Christ, Wagner, & Stellmacher, 2007), and to explore the multiple dynamics that characterize the persons from groups who have less perceived power or worth (Dixon, Tropp, Durrheim, & Tredoux, 2010).
Goals of the Current Study
In the current study, we used core tenets of the contact hypothesis to explore Muslim women’s connection between contact with others, having once arrived and settled into a new country, and their perceived wellbeing. We specifically included women who moved to the United States after having lived in a country where Islam is a dominant religion or where Muslims constitute a majority of the population. This approach provided to us the unique ability to glean the perspectives of women who, in their native countries, were members of a dominant group (i.e., in terms of the contact hypothesis, were among the ingroup) and, in moving to the United States where Islam is not a dominant religion, then became a member of a minority group (i.e., an outgroup). This purposive sample allowed us to explore how contact with others was related to women’s perceptions of social, community, and work relationships. The contact hypothesis is predicated on the belief that, in interacting with persons who are different from persons in someone’s prior community or social network, assumptions about perceived differences across groups can be challenged and beliefs about fundamental differences can be minimized or extinguished (that is, moving from an Us-Them to a We orientation). Thus, we sought to explore how women perceived the tenuous dynamic of how building or co-creating relationships in a new country.
We used a qualitative-quantitative mixed-design (Creswell, 2014), and, in doing so, we were able to record participants’ experience both in terms of established measures of psychosocial adjustment as well as women’s personal narratives. This design allowed us to explore the ways in which women could narrate the juxtaposition of having lived most of her life in an area where her religion – and often her ethnicity – was in the majority and where, in a new country the same religion is far less common and frequently is enshrouded with fearmongering and falsehoods. Using women’s biographic narratives, we explored how Muslim women perceived, interpreted, and adapted to a substantially different cultural living context as they connected with persons in new and vastly different geographical and cultural environment.
Method
Participants
This study used a mixed-methods design relying on purposive sampling. In purposive sampling, participants are chosen or invited into the study because they meet specific criteria. When purposive sampling is used within qualitative-based studies, the sample size can be small; sample size, by itself, is not a salient concern in qualitative studies if the sample size allows for data saturation. Thirteen women were included in this study. In order to participate in the study, all women met four criteria. First, all women self-identified as Muslim. Second, all women moved to the United States after living in a Muslim-centered country. We used sampling maximization for this criterion and did not specify how long women had to have lived in a Muslim-centered country prior to immigrating to the U.S.; one woman moved to the United States with her family as an adolescent and the remainder of women moved to the U.S. as adults. All women were drawn from the local community and were able to travel to an interview room at a nearby university. Third, women understood that they would be asked to complete quantitative measures (that is, established and validated scales measuring a variety of domains, such as indices of adjustment, faith- and spirituality-based measures) which were printed in English. And, finally, all women were willing to be audio-/video-recorded and double confirmed on the consent form that their biographic interviews would be transcribed for analysis. All data were collected during the summer of 2014.
Women ranged in age from 21-37 with a mean age of 26.4 years. Three women identified themselves as Asian/Southeast Asian, three as African/black, six as Arab or Middle Eastern, and one as Caucasian/white.[2] Four of the women were single (never married) and nine were married. Twelve of the women reported growing up in a nuclear family with both biological parents in the household; one woman reported growing up with one biological parent and one step-parent. All women grew up with siblings, ranging from two to ten siblings; the modal number was two siblings. All women had some college education. In terms of daily living, we asked informants about their current education and/or employment. Seven women possessed a baccalaureate degree, four were currently matriculated into a graduate or post-baccalaureate training program, and two women were in a baccalaureate program. One informant reported that she was a full-time homemaker. Two worked full time and two worked part time; three were unemployed. Five women were attending university full time. Six women relied on public transit systems for local travel (e.g., from their home to university or for errands), two relied on a relative, four drove a vehicle themselves, and one informant reported that she mostly chose to walk as her preferred mode of transportation.
Women were asked, on a 4-point, Likert-type scale how important their religion was to them. One woman reported that her faith was important, and twelve women reported that their faith was very important. In terms of religious service attendance, four women reported that they attended mosques regularly and nine women reported that they did not. What might account for this discrepancy in the high value informants placed on their faith yet only a third of women accessed a mosque regularly? The answer is geography. The women lived in a small city located in a desert mountain area with a population of around 68,000 persons. There is no mosque in the area. The nearest mosque is in a city two- to three- hours (by car travel) away; thus, women may have wanted to attend a mosque more frequently than they did but the challenges of travel, especially during winter months, precluded more frequent religious service attendance. Nine participants wore hijab and four did not. None of the women were born in the United States; all migrated to the U.S., usually for advanced academic study – either for themselves or their husband – although three women migrated by themselves and three with their family of origin. Most respondents migrated to the U.S. 2 years prior to their participation in the study, although one woman migrated with her family when she was an adolescent. On average, both the mean and modal ages for women moving to the U.S. was 22 years.
Procedure
The study focused on the lives of Muslim women who moved to the United States after having lived most of their lives in a Muslim-centered country and now lived in the U.S., in an era in which anti-Muslim sentiment is common. The study was approved by the university’s Institutional Review Board. The second author made contact with participants, explained the purposes of the study, and asked if they would be interested in participating in the study. Participants were interviewed convenient to their schedules. In qualitative studies, establishing rapport with participants is seen not only as respectful of participants’ time and generosity in sharing their experience, but also as a form of procedural validity. Rapport was built through in-person contact, email, or phone correspondence in establishing a convenient time for her participation in the study, and in meeting each woman and escorting her to the interview room.[3] Women completed their interview and completion of a survey packet individually. Upon arriving at the research location, women were greeted by the research assistant and escorted to the interview room. Participants were presented with two identical consent forms and asked to read and sign both forms. One form was retained by the research assistant and the other was returned to participants. Forms contained all contact information for both the research assistant (and interviewer), the project faculty supervisor, and the Institutional Review Board office. Participants were encouraged to call any member of the university should they have follow-up questions. Throughout the course of study no adverse events were reported by any participant. Participants were remunerated with a $20 gift card to either a national retail store or a national-chain grocery store (they could choose which card they preferred).
The study took place in a university office that had been transformed into a professional interviewing room. Women were told both at recruitment and through the informed consent process that there were two portions to the study: the completion of a questionnaire and a taped interview. Once informed consent forms were completed, a questionnaire was presented. At this point the interviewer left the room so that each woman could complete her questionnaire privately. Although the questionnaire items did not contain items which we thought were especially personal, we believed participants may be more comfortable completing the questionnaire without another person in the room. Once completed, women placed the survey into an envelope, and then informed the interviewer – who was seated down the hall from the interview room – that they had finished the survey. The questionnaire remained in the envelope during the interview phase of the study; this was an intentional element to the research protocol. The researchers wanted to convey to informants that their responses to questionnaires were both confidential and wholly independent from their subsequent biographic interview. When each participant informed the interviewer that she had completed the questionnaire, she was asked if she would like to proceed directly to the interview or if she would like a short break; all participants were offered a soft drink, juice, or water bottle prior to the start of the interview.
The qualitative portion of the study contained interview questions designed to situate informants in the broader context as a Muslim woman living in the United States. Informants were reminded that the interview was audio- and video-recorded; women were informed two recordings were made in case there was a mechanical failure with one data feed. Both on the consent form and by the interviewer, participants were informed that once verbatim transcriptions of the interview were completed and checked for reliability, audio and video recordings would be deleted. Verbatim transcriptions were created from recordings. Twelve interviews produced usable digital files. The audio data from one participant– someone who was completely engaged throughout the interview – did not register with either the audio recorder (mounted on the ceiling directly above where the woman and the interviewer were seated) or the audio-video feed (mounted on a office desk directly in front of her). The digital video feed showed the participant engaged in the interview (gesturing with her hands, smiling) but, with an inaudible voice such that no sound was recorded. We brought in an expert from the institution’s Information Technology division hoping a professional with more advanced equipment could extract the audio feed loud enough such that transcriptions could be made, but that was also unsuccessful.
Transcriptions were created within three days of each interview. Subsequently, a research assistant unrelated to the study checked the accuracy of transcriptions. Any discrepancies were discussed by the transcriptionist and supervising faculty member. Few discrepancies occurred and, when they did, they focused on portions of audio text where some words seemed less than fully clear. In the end, reliability checks of transcriptions exceeded 95%, establishing what is referred to within qualitative research as synchronic reliability (Kirk & Miller, 1986). Because the study used a qualitative-quantitative mixed-design, and some qualitative data were coded into quantitative codes to merge data, the results presented here are based on the twelve women for whom we have both complete quantitative and (biographic) narrative data.
Measures
Quantitative Measures
Depressive Symptomatology. The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) is a 20-item, standardized self-report instrument. Items (scored on a scale of 0 to 3) describe the frequency of depressive symptoms (e.g., “I was bothered by things that normally don’t bother me,” “I felt depressed”) in the prior seven days. In the current sample, Cronbach alpha was .60, and the mean was 12.77 (SD = 5.41, range = 5 - 23). A CES-D score of 22 or above indicates that an individual may be at risk for more sustained depressive symptoms; two of the women in this study fell into this category, but only slightly.
Social Support. Participants’ access to available social support was measured by the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS; Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, 1988). The MSPSS reliably measures individuals’ perceptions to their access of social support. The scale is comprised of 12 items, ranging from 1 (Very strongly disagree) to 7 (Very strongly agree). We used the full scale, which accesses perceptions of social support from family, friends, and significant others. In the current sample, the Cronbach alpha measuring internal consistency of items was .83; the overall scale mean was 6.14 (SD = .61, range 5-7).
Optimism. Our third indicant of adjustment included a measure of dispositional optimism as assessed by the Life Orientation Test (LOT; Scheier & Carver, 1985). Dispositional optimism is believed to be a robust personality construct that predicts if persons expect to have positive or negative experiences in life and can serve as a protective (or buffer) factor in the face of life stressors (Andersson, 1996). The full scale contains 12 items, although 4 are filler items; thus, the scale is based on 8 items using a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree). Scores thus range from 8 to 35, which higher scores suggesting greater optimism; in the current sample, Cronbach alpha = .65. The LOT mean for this sample was 27.08 (SD = 2.39; range 24-31).
Spiritual Beliefs. In order to assess participants’ spiritual (but not religious-specific beliefs), we
used the Spiritual Beliefs Inventory (SRI-15R; Holland et al., 1998). The Spiritual Beliefs Inventory measures both beliefs and practices with items such as “When I need suggestions on how to deal with problems, I know someone in my religious or spiritual community that I can turn to,” and “I have experienced peace of mind through prayers and meditation.” Holland and colleagues (1998) provide ample evidence for the psychometric strengths associated with this scale. Scores can range from 15-60 with higher scores reflecting greater perceived spiritual support from personal faith practices and connections to a faith community. The Cronbach was strong (alpha = .81); in the current sample, scores ranged from 42 to 60 with a mean of 53.62 (SD = 4.59).
As a secondary index of spirituality, we also assessed participants’ strength of faith using the Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith Questionnaire (Plante & Boccaccini,1997). This measure adds an important dimension to spirituality that is less salient in the Spiritual Beliefs Inventory. While the SRI-15 focuses on individual practices and community support, the Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith Questionnaire focuses on the degree to which faith is personality important to individuals. Items include “I look to my faith as providing meaning and purpose in my life” and “I look to my faith as a source of comfort.” This scale is measured in a Likert-type scale from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 4 (Strongly Agree) with higher scores indicating stronger self-perceived strength of faith. Scores can thus range from 10-40. In the current sample, scores ranged between 28 - 40; the mean in this sample was 36.85 (SD = 3.41) and internal consistency strong (Cronbach alpha = .82).
Islamic Reflection. As a measure of participants’ orientation toward Islam, we used the 14 items from the Islamic Reflections Scale (Dover, Milner, & Dowson, 2007). This measure was developed to assess multiple components to how Muslims conceptualize and internalize faith and is based on the assumption that, as with other prevalent religious faiths, Muslims see interconnections between Islam and related facets of contemplative spirituality. Although participants completed the entire measure, we were specifically interested in the subscale Dover and colleagues called Reflective Commitment, a dimension demonstrating “a thought-out commitment to their faith” (2007, p. 194) which was seen by its authors as orthogonal to stereotyped assumptions about Muslims as rigid in an adherence to Islam. The 14 items are measured on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Strong Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree) and can range between 14-70. In our sample, scores ranged from 53-66 (M = 61.23, SD = 4.08). The Cronbach alpha for the Reflective Commitment subscale was .89.
Religious-based Prejudice. To measure participants’ perceptions that they have experienced or witnessed others’ prejudicial attitudes or discriminatory treatment based on their being identified as Muslim, we used the Perceived Religious Discrimination Scale (Rippy & Newman, 2008) which was adapted from a separate measure of race-related stress. This scale is measured on a scale of 1 (Never) to 5 (Very frequently) on a range of items reflecting differential treatment attributable to being identified as Muslim. The scale includes items such as “Have you felt your presence at work or school was resented because you were Muslim?” and “Do you ever feel that you do not fit in with larger American culture?” The full scale contains 33 items which can be divided into three subscales. In the current study, we used Religious Prejudice and Stigmatization (22 items, Cronbach = .88) and Exposure to Religiously Discriminatory Environment (6 items, Cronbach = .85). Five items are loaded onto a subscale Rippy and Newman (2008) identify as Bicultural Identification and Conflict. These items were unrelated to our study and thus we eliminated these 5 items from the scale, leaving a measure of 28 items. Scores on this 28-item measure can range between 28 and 140 with lower scores indicating a reduced perception of discrimination or the experience of prejudice based on being Muslim. The Cronbach alpha coefficient for the full 28 items in the current sample was .91. Scores ranged from 22-55 (M = 33.69, SD = 10.59).
Results
One reason to use a mixed design is capture different domains of persons’ experiences. For example, in some studies, surveys – which assess attitudes or values – certainly could be asked via a one-on-one interview, but respondents’ survey-oriented data are often seen as more complete and internally valid if responses can be recorded privately (Creswell, 2014). On the other hand, narrative experience – life stories or the turning points that characterize a life event, for example – are often benefitted by using a different methodology, such as an interview. Multiple methods – such as those in a qualitative-quantitative design – allow one type of data to inform another (e.g., a score on a scale measuring depression can be contextualized with an interview that asks someone to speak to what depression feels like). In this study, the sample size met the threshold of data saturation (Charmaz, 2014; Corbin & Strauss, 2008; Strauss & Corbin, 1990). For the qualitative data, our sample size was more than adequate. For the quantitative data, the sample size is small and this precluded a number of statistical analyses. Instead, we see the value of our quantitative data as providing a richer characterization of our participants’ experiences and in complementing their narratives.
Quantitative Analyses
We queried the women in our sample on three indices of psychosocial adjustment: depressive symptoms, dispositional optimism, and perceptions of accessible social support. We compared their overall scores to population data. Participants did not differ from the general population in terms of depressive symptoms (z = -.15, ns) or optimism (z = 1.62, ns) and, in fact, reported slightly more social support than is generally seen in the population, z = 3.34, p < .006. As is typically found in population-based samples, greater optimism was related to decreased depression, r -.56. Taken together, it appears that the women in our sample were, on the whole, very similar to the general population in terms of three indices of adjustment. These three indices were unrelated to measures assessing women’s spirituality or perceptions of discrimination based on being Muslim.
Respondents’ self-reports evidence strong personal dedication to faith. Summary descriptive information was reported above. On two scales measuring components of spirituality – the Spiritual Beliefs Inventory and the Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith measure – mean responses were near the ceiling of both scales and the range of scores were in the upper two quartiles. On a scale specific to Islam, participants demonstrated moderately high scores generally and on the Reflective Commitment subscale – assessing the extent to which respondents had thought through competing religious orthodoxies and chosen Islam based on their own will – the scores were also high (range 7-15, M = 12.7, SD = 2.28). In fact, the Religious Commitment subscale of the Islamic Reflection Scale, but not the overall scale, was positively associated both with the Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith measure ( r = .73, p < .005) and the Spiritual Beliefs Inventory ( r = .82. p < .001). These associations suggest the women in our sample reported a strong sense of spiritual faith but neither (given the lack of associations with other components of the Islamic Reflection Scale) spiritual/religious rigidity or dogmatism. Generally speaking, most participants did not perceive much prejudice or discrimination attributable to their status as being Muslim in a nonMuslim-dominant country. Although the two subscales we measured – Religious Prejudice/Stigmatization and Religiously Discriminatory Environment – were related to one another, r = .65, p < .05), the overall descriptive data do not suggest participants felt they had experienced even moderate levels of prejudice or discrimination. The two scales assessing prejudice and discrimination were not associated with the spirituality/Islamic Reflection measures, all rs < .49, ns.
Interpretive Analyses
Qualitative analyses stemmed from normative coding procedures. Codes were identified from one-third of verified transcripts. A codebook was developed to identify and define codes. There can be multiple purposes for codes but codes routinely identify the core issues articulated by participants and mark sections of narrative text that allow for analytical indexing (Hennink, Hutter, & Bailey, 2015). Interpretive analyses congeal around both inductive codes (that is, themes articulated by informants) and by deductive codes established with research questions. Qualitative researchers often refer to participants in interview or focus group studies as informants (cf., subjects); contextualist paradigms believe that persons are the authorities on their own experience and inform researchers about their lived experience. Coding organizes what can be an enormous amount of textual data into categories. We followed standard coding procedures (Corbin & Strauss, 2008; Charmaz, 2014; Strauss, 1994; Strauss & Corbin, 1990) by using open coding and axial coding (Hennink et al., 2015).
Recall the women in this study were purposively sampled because they had moved from a country where Islam was a dominant religion to a country where Islam is a minority religion. Irrespective of their native countries (including countries in Africa, Asia, and the Middle East) all informants expressed some trepidation about moving to the United States. Women stated that media representations – including feature films but mainly news outlets – posture the United States as non-welcoming to many persons, but especially to persons who are African, from any country designated as “Arab,” and Muslims. Some informants’ families sought to restrain women’s efforts to travel to the United States even when the pursuance of a college or graduate-level education (one woman received a Fulbright award) was seen as valuable both to the informant and her community (assuming she would return to her home country). Warnings often included perceptions of the ways in which women could be mistreated in the United States (e.g., stemming from racism or derision toward Muslims) but also included a perception that, in moving to the United States, women would become changed in ways that would impede their adjustment if she returned to her native or another Muslim-dominant country. One informant put it this way:
Well, if you go – I learn a lot here, you know? In my country they say, if you leave your country you will be a different person. You will get money. You will get a lot of [pause] what do you call it? Knowledge. You will know a lot of stuff. And if you go back you will always be different. You will not behave like you used to. Right now, even when I’m talking to my dad [via Skype, FaceTime] he says, you cannot come back.
All women reported having received some caution about moving to the United States. The daily living freedoms perceived to be allocated to persons residing in the United States were especially concerning to some informants’ friends and family members.
Across countries of origin, women reported alarmist news imagery about the United States. Uniformly, informants attributed anti-United States sentiment to the sequelae of the 9/11 terrorist attacks. All women expressed either empathy for the victims of the attacks or scorn for the persons who executed the attacks. Informants expressed sorrow and anger at how the 9/11 attacks were summarily yoked to Americans’ suspicion of both members of Islam and persons from the Middle Eastern countries. Some informants reported they were suspicious of the media coverage they received in their home countries and three-quarters of the women recognized – often after having migrated to the U.S. – that countries have their own (often unstated) goals of propelling or dissuading the population’s interest in other countries’ political, gender, and religious structures.
I was scared to come here. I was thinking, if I come here, American persons [pause] they don’t like Black, African, Islam. It’s too hard for them. I didn’t want to come. Later I realized it’s [pause] it’s all lies.
This informant had received pressure not to accompany her husband to the United States as he pursued advanced study and she saw the warnings she heard about potential mistreatment as a parallel example of the very kind of prejudice her country postured Americans as displaying.
Some informants did experience negative – rude or dismissive – interactions with some Americans as they traveled from their home countries to the U.S. Specifically, the majority of women cited treatment from airport employees that targeted Muslims as security threats which (subsequently) warranted additional searches. One woman stated that she removed her hijab during international travel specifically to avoid harassment. Another woman asked an airline employee why she was being retained and questioned by airline staff and was told, quite pragmatically, staff had been warned about persons traveling from her native country. One woman’s brother told her that, if she wore a hijab at a point of U.S. entry, they would not allow her in.
I know they fear Muslims. [They think] ahh, maybe a terrorist. So when I was going [preparing to leave Africa] my younger brother was telling me, you have to remove the hijab. They will send you back to [home country]. Why you trust Americans? They will send you back. Don’t put on hijab there. You won’t get a job. You won’t get anything. You’ll be isolated. So I had to remove the hijab.
Women’s experiences with traveling to the U.S. had the potential to cement the cautionary tales they heard. But women reported how they (and their husbands) were treated in airports was unlike how they were treated by new colleagues, teachers, and neighbors once they settled in a new community.
Getting to Know You: Contact with Americans
Other than the women who were married, and either traveled with their husband or alone to join him, no women in the study knew of anyone else in the city where they moved to and established residency. Some women had family or acquaintances in other U.S. cities and, of these women, a couple visited with others if their travel itinerary included stops where their family resided (mostly in New York City or Chicago). However, once they arrived to their new home city, they knew no one (other than, again, those women who were married). Thus, having been primed to be curious – if not suspicious – of the United States, U.S. citizens, and American culture, and with some having disquieting travel experiences perceived as attributable to being Muslim, women were tasked with developing new relationships. In addition, because, as we reviewed earlier, the city where these women resided has no mosque or established religious community for Muslims, most of the persons women met were persons with whom they perceived they might have little in common.
Informants’ initial transition to living in the United States was reported as easier than they had anticipated. Nine women spoke specifically about adjusting to daily freedoms accorded to U.S. citizens and to them as new residents (e.g., walking to university alone, obtaining a driver’s license, and purchasing their own groceries without a brother or father to escort them), and ten women spoke positively about how principles of democracy structured daily living. One woman stated that she was grateful for laws that served to protect persons, such as traffic laws and car seats for children. Even for women who wore hijab, the choice was seen as personally meaningful. Informants spoke to their appreciation and gratitude for governmentally based democracies and how they hoped at least some basic elements of democratic governance could be adopted by other countries. Perhaps the most effusive of these comments was by one woman who migrated to the U.S. from a country that had been engaged in a sustained war.
I feel more relaxed over here. Yeah because the conditions in [native country] are really bad. And you know, um, all the time you have threats on your life. There are, you know, bombs and all that stuff. And terrorists of course. So you know, nobody’s safe over there. No one is safe. So now I feel relaxed. We plan to stay here as long as, you know, our visa goes on. So after that, if my husband gets a job over there, then we’ll continue. Otherwise we will go to another country like Turkey or [pause] some other country, yeah, any other country. You know when someone comes to U.S. nobody wants to go back. Whether he’s from Pakistan, from India, from China, from anywhere else. I believe that people don’t want to go back, but if conditions are good in your country, so you know, that’s the best place for you. But when, you know, you feel, you know, threat of life, like your kids are not safe. They are – if you’re going on the road – there is a blast so you can die. If your kids are going to school, there’s an attack on the school. So that’s why, you know, I think the U.S. is better to live in.
According to this informant, it took some time to adjust to the fact the totality of daily living is quite safe: generally, in the United States, persons do not worry about a bomb explosion, an attack on their child’s school, or military personnel barging into a person’s residence. Although across a few informants there seemed to be rose-covered memories of their home countries (e.g., one woman spoke to how peaceful her country was, how violence was not common at any level of community life; this woman’s memories were juxtaposed to ongoing conflict in her home country televised across various international news channels), all informants demonstrated an appreciation for living in the United States where they described relationships with their neighbors, school associates, and coworkers as mostly harmonious.
As immigrants, the women in our sample reported that they wanted to learn about Americans and daily living practices. They also reported that they felt this was easy to do because, overwhelmingly, women participants perceived persons in their community as friendly, helpful, curious, and kind. Only two women reported any negative experiences. In one circumstance, a male college student made a negative comment about the participant’s friend’s home country (and the country the student castigated was, in fact, nowhere near where the woman was from). In the second case, one woman believed that she had received unfair treatment in relation to a job. This informant, however, said repeatedly that she did not believe the mistreatment she experienced was attributable to her being Muslim but rather of her being black (and did express a belief that Americans, including at times black Americans, are racially biased toward black immigrants). But by far, most informants felt they could befriend others easily because society’s openness was made available to them.
You know, one thing I like about the U.S.: the people are really friendly. And they don’t interfere with your life. That’s the best thing about the U.S. We are free to wear our own dresses, we are free to wear our hijab – nobody says, nobody says anything to you. Only in the airport. [There] we got checked, you know, thoroughly and were said few bad words. Generally people are really good, yeah. We are, you know, free here. We are not threatened. We are not called terrorist. Yeah, I have never been called terrorist, thank God. But I’m happy about that. I’m happy about that. Yeah, we are free to offer our prayers. We are free to practice whatever we want to. In my neighborhood, I have you know, some Spanish friends. I have you know some English people from U.S. Some Native Americans. Yeah, I have friends.
And, from another informant,
Uhh, so, no, I haven’t faced any challenges here. Maybe because I’m not covered. Umm, but, you know when, for example, all of my friends know that we are Muslims but umm, I didn’t feel that they have some [pause] barriers to talk with us. I always feel myself comfortable uhh, among other, other people who are not Muslims. Who are Christian, for example. Because me and my husband have a lot of friends who are not Muslims, and they’re always inviting us and we’re having time together, and it’s really good. They are interested in our religions. So, ahh, for example just when we are having dinner. And when we are, for example, we’re not eating the usual meat here, we’re always looking for halal meat. And they’re interested: Why? What’s the difference between, umm, common meat and halal meat? And like this. Just common, common questions. [In summarizing] Yeah. As I told you, you are like. In the first place, you are a person first, not your religion. Because I’m proud that I’m Muslim and, umm, that’s why I don’t mind about this [acquaintances and friends being curious about Islamic practices]. And, also, I see that people are friendly to me and they’re not like, ignoring me because of – because I am Muslim.
And, from a third informant who reflected on how she felt prior to arriving in the United States and what her experience has been after less than a year of interacting with local community members:
Before I came to the United States I am afraid. Maybe a lot of people fear me or hurt me. Because you know, the media change our things [gestures to her skull to convey a change in attitudes]. Before I came to the United States I think the American – all American people – hate the Muslims. But when I came to the United States I feel that’s a wrong idea. Most of the people respect me and respect my hijab, and I think that I am very surprised when I saw the people very friendly to [pause] with me. And when I go to the store or the market, all the Americans [as other shoppers] and the employees help me and when I ask them any questions they give me what I want. And I think [pause] sometimes I feel they help all the people, like I’m an American. It is very good. They, uhh, they are very friendly. And also, I put my daughter in the daycare and the women there they take care of her. They are very friendly and good, and my daughter like him [them]. They like my daughter and they feel comfortable. I put my daughter with him [them]. They good people.
The women in our sample shared the limitations of living in a small city (no easy access to a mosque or halal meat, the vast difference between the city sizes between where they moved from and where they lived now), but they also narrated how, in their experience, the smaller city provided a fuller opportunity for recent immigrants to meet and interact with Americans in ways that were less available in large cities. For example, those informants who had been to large U.S. cities (Atlanta, Los Angeles) commented on how the city size allows for minority populations to segregate into their own neighborhoods; this paves the way for increased contact with persons with whom one is similar but can simultaneously impede the chances to interact with persons dissimilar from self and family. These comments align with the contact hypothesis: It is the process of interacting with persons dissimilar from oneself that provides opportunity to revise attitudes and opinions about the outgroup. These women moved to a small city where either they and/or their husbands were embedded into new social systems: neighborhoods, university courses, university teaching positions, parent groups. Although women did befriend other Muslims, the majority of their social contacts were persons who had been raised in a vastly different cultural context and whose spiritual or religious identity was unlikely to be seen in the same way as was the religious identity of the informants. Women’s social interactions with persons from the dominant cultural group were perceived by women to crumble prior assumptions about Americans. Although women reported appreciating features of dominant U.S. society (e.g., freedoms both in daily living and in personal identities, opportunities for women), informants uniformly reported that interacting with Americans – those in the dominant outgroup – challenged previously held ascriptions of Americans.
The position of establishing a new life in a new country pushed women to revising concepts of themselves. One informant spoke of how adjusting to what she saw as the American value of independence led to her feel better about herself. In response to an interview probe on what the informant saw as a primary challenge to adjusting to a new country, one woman said:
Ahh, the biggest change. I have a broader view. A broader view. And, umm, I become more self-sufficient. And depend on myself for everything. I like that because I usually depend on my family. So, I feel stronger here. When I meet other people they told me their cultural beliefs and I say, oh that’s [pause] maybe it’s good. Like I have awareness in my mind. I become more flexible because it’s a lot of different people. America. I think, uhh, maybe I can’t understand a lot of people and the people don’t understand me. And I think they are different. But now. I feel that we are all the same.
For this informant, interacting with persons from a new country was seen as an opportunity for individual development. All of our informants reported an opportunity to revise and reconstruct their opinions on the ways in which both the United States and the persons living in the United States were framed in their own country’s post-9/11 discourses. Among all participants, assumptions of religious prejudice by Americans was revised. The far majority of respondents perceived that Americans were respectful of their historical and cultural backgrounds, and saw befriending informants as an opportunity for persons to learn from one another.
Contact with other Muslims
The women in this study reported advantages for the opportunities to interact with Americans once they moved from their countries of origin to a small city in the United States. Informants often reported that they discussed religion with non-Muslim American acquaintances and friends, and they were as interested in others’ faiths as their friends were with their Islam. Two women specifically spoke to how they went to (Christian) church with friends specifically to learn about their friends’ religious beliefs and practices. Before living in the U.S., one informant lived in a northern European country which she identified as very Catholic and, while she was living there, she twice read the Bible so that she could understand the culture and people with whom she lived. Two other participants narrated friendships with persons they identified as Mormon; these women found it both surprising and rather reassuring that they saw similarities across tenets of both the Mormon Church and Islam.
The participants met and befriended other Muslims – mainly persons who also who migrated to the U.S. for purposes of advanced study or in accompanying their husbands – from a variety of other countries. These interactions were perceived by women as fundamental in expanding their own reflected commitment to religious faith. Before moving to the U.S., three women reported travel experiences outside of their home countries. Thus, for the majority of participants, their experience with Islam, and enacted performances of tradition, were intricately conjoined with how their particular home community – or, in some cases, the country – presented and practiced Islam. Informants reported an intense interest in the underlying cultural factors that may have created societies’ different practices of religion. For example, one woman appreciated the fact she learned from other Muslim women that she could wear clothing of different colors, as in her country she reported that women only wear the color black.
Informants reported feeling as if they learned nearly as much from Muslims who immigrated from other countries as they did from Americans. Although they could identify with some or even many religious aspects of other Muslims, participants also reported that different interpretations of Islam impacted the ways in which they reflected on and incorporated dimensions of faith into their own lives. Respondents reported being grateful (and for those who were married, including their husbands) for the chance to meet and interact with other Muslims in part simply because of the smaller number of Muslims in this area of the U.S. Relationships with other Muslims benefitted women both socially (e.g., an informal prayer group meeting) and practically (e.g., someone who was traveling to the state’s largest city, where familiar foods and halal meats are available, could bring items back for others). The social contact with Muslims from other countries provided a conversational opportunity about how Islam is embedded into larger societal culture that most women said they never had (because most had not traveled prior to moving to the U.S.).
Participants keenly recognized how the societal context (of living in the United States) permitted choices in religious life. That is, informants reporting believing that the absolute adherence to religious expectations in some countries stemmed directly from only some interpretations of fidelity to Islam. But because they met Muslims from other countries, where religious practices were different, women came to see that what they at first thought were tenets of their religion were, instead, dynamics of different culture. The expansive freedoms afforded to persons in the U.S. were understood by women as an unanticipated gift to engage in dialogue with other Muslims about the diversity of religious practice. According to one informant:
Being Muslim [here] ... just you have a chance to learn, to know the differences between culture and Islam, you know? Because, when I was home [in native country] there was a lot of stuff that I used to believe is related to Islam, that when I came here I see like, I see different people, from different country, different Muslims. I realize those things are not Islam, but it’s – like it’s the culture. I am around different Muslims from different countries, so that’s how I learned.
When asked by the interviewer to describe the conversations this informant has with Muslims that she has met from other countries, she responded:
Yeah, we argue [discuss] a lot, about Islam. Some people said that this [discussions about religion] is against Islam, some people just say that’s not against Islam. Yeah, we learn a lot. We just discuss stuff between us, about Islam. Anything we want to talk about, with Islam, we talk about. Anything you want to learn, you can find it.
This woman’s experience, echoed by others, delineates how she was given the chance to question and learn by having contact with Muslims who she previously would not have had the chance to know.
Frequently informants reported feeling validated by learning how they or their friends may have been treated as a result of their identity as Muslims is not dissimilar to how persons of other faiths have been treated. A participant, for example, was able to draw parallels between how two of her friends – one Mormon, one Muslim – reported how they perceived they had been treated by others which they attributed to their religious identities.
Well, for example, my friend from Argentina. She’s Mormon. And, I don’t know, they’re similar [to Muslims]. People don’t like Mormons, you know. So she had a lot of, she went through a lot of, you know, discrimination from Argentina before coming to Arizona. So she had a lot of discrimination. She told me a lot of things that went on, you know, how they just didn’t like Mormons. And, just like, I felt like I could relate to her because I went through the same thing as her, you know? And, umm, also my friend from Afghanistan, like, umm, a lot of people [in the U.S.] really, umm, she was one of the friends who was physically harmed. Like this guy just came up to her and pushed her around and – he was drunk, I found out he was drunk, still no excuse. But yeah, just knowing their stories, or what they went through, and like, cause they’re from a different culture, and different from where we are here. And, like, they all went through similar things that I went through. So that just, you know, kinda brought us closer.
Similarly, some women found the camaraderie of befriending and interacting with other Muslims as an opportunity to personally strengthen elements of their own interpretation of Islam. This was mentioned by half of the women in our sample and is illustrated in this example reported by a participant who, working in a Muslim-affiliated group, performed social outreach.
I’m part of this Islamic club. We like, we like, we feed the homeless every Friday. So, like, we post videos on YouTube just to show, like what we do. We’re not all about like protesting and being against America, like we’re American citizens. We live here. We’re one of you guys. Just because we dress differently doesn’t mean we’re different from you guys. We’re Americans just like you. Just if like you are Irish, you know, originally. And you have different cultures, different food, and you don’t dress differently. But you continue your culture, your religion. But we decided to continue the culture [pause] we have our culture, just continue with it, while still being away from home. But we’re the same people [as non-Muslims]. We’re still a good people and we go to poverty camps just to help out and feed the homeless. We like post videos and show people what we are all about.
The freedoms of association and religion, long-ingrained fundamental belief systems potentially taken for granted by Americans, were seen by a few participants as providing opportunities to reflect on religious practice. One informant who, through her job with a university, interacted with a number of international students originating from the Middle East. This informant positioned herself as well-liked by the students she worked with and, consequently, students disclosed to her – both privately and in the context of group discussions – how they saw living in the U.S. as a reprieve from the rigid regulations of their home countries. In the U.S., students could engage in behavior that would be prohibited at home. According to this informant, this included (for women) not wearing hijab, (for men) having multiple romantic relationships, and (potentially for both women and men) engaging in casual sexual relationships. This informant clearly felt students’ behavior was a result of not being monitored by a government system or social structure yoked to religion.
And they will discuss with me. To me, it’s totally different because [in informants’ home country] it’s totally illegal – that’s one thing. And, on the other hand, Islamic rules are interpreted according to the time you’re living and also [by] culture. They are different. And many do not pray at all. And [pause] I can understand this because they got pressure from their countries and they feel free here to do whatever they want. You have to believe something in your heart. It’s, it’s not what enforcement is or it’s not something you can practice without believing. There are so many guys and they don’t care. They’re with Russian girls, hugging them and stuff. I believe they are [pause] they have no authority over them now, who’s controlling them. They just feel, like, okay this is, this is a contemporary period of my life. I should, I should have fun. And no one questions me, so I will live like that. And that’s understandable.
The informant continued to speak of a man fairly well known among students. According to the informant, the man is in the U.S. alone and his wife and children are in his home country:
... he’s married and he has two children and he’s thirty-five or something. And he started dating an American woman, and they started living together and we started seeing them together everywhere, holding hands. And I have many friends they were making fun of him because he is Muslim, he’s married, and he’s doing this. And that bothers me a lot because then people are gonna think that all the Muslim guys are this way. But actually, that’s not true. The system is so strict in their country. But here, they come here and they’re cheating. I mean, in that sense, like their country is so strict but in the United States they can do whatever they like.
This informant did not begrudge access to the many freedoms international students had available to them while living in the U.S. She identified herself as from a country that is known as having a Westernized-like democratic government where citizens are afforded more civil freedoms. Rather, the informant was concerned that without having internalized some principles of living one’s faith, and modeling faith practices in response to home countries’ strict rules of allegiance, some persons’ behavior flaunted behavior that was inconsistent with Islamic principles.
Social Contact and Faith Commitments
In this study, we merged central components of the contact hypothesis with a qualitative design. We assumed that our informants’ social and collegial relationships with persons they met in the United States would be perceived as important contributors to their experience living in a new, very different country and one where proportionately few persons share the same faith. Participants narrated the parameters of their relationships with Americans and almost without exception reported positive experiences. They also shared how their interactions with other Muslims, persons who also were from countries which were steeped in their own beliefs and practices. These relationships offered informants social support, but they also did something much more: Discussions about Islam with other Muslims who had learned about Islam differently, and who practiced Islam through different cultural practices, compelled our informants to reflect on how they wanted to incorporate Islam into their worldview and the daily practices.
By living in the United States and by interacting with Muslims from across the world, participants perceived both a freedom and an opportunity to talk about, learn from, and intellectually expand their understanding of Islam. The majority of respondents reported that learning different interpretations and customs associated with Islam taught them both the breadth of Islamic tradition and permitted them to disentangle their own particular country’s or community’s displays of faith from other, equally legitimate enactments of faith. Although they felt the ability – the freedom – to question and challenge their own legacies with Islam, all participants felt this questioning and contemplative journey was beneficial, and no woman in our study reported losing faith or lessening religious commitment from engaging in a reflective process. Instead, they perceived that the process of being able to question their faith (and for some participants, without a fear of penalty) had the effect of strengthening their understanding of and commitment to Islam. One informant spoke to it thus:
Well I think it’s their right to have freedom. Everybody need to have freedom. As a Muslim I feel [pause] I feel like [pause] I talk to people in country where they are forced to cover their hair and, ahh, I don’t believe in that. Like maybe they should not do such things [requirement]. They need to learn by themselves, not force. Because at home they just tell you stuff. You don’t get a chance ... I did not get a chance to see them on my own perspective. So I was just doing stuff because I have to. I didn’t know why. So here, when I came here, I get chance to learn, and know why I’m feeling this, what is true in Islam. I learn a lot. I don’t want, like, to have to wear hijab because they told me to wear it. I don’t know why. So I think it’s very important to question why [I do this] you know? So, if you are from the culture where the majority is Muslim, in most cases, you don’t know why. You just do it. But if you leave there – if you came, you have – you are free. You could take off your hijab if you wanted. You could do anything, you know? But then you realize the truth. Yeah. I think you have more faith if you decide.
When this informant’s interview had concluded and she was talking casually with the interviewer, she revisited her opinions on the benefits of allowing persons across faiths to explore what was important to them within and across religious orthodoxies. She mentioned how learning from Muslim friends, who she had met while living in the U.S., gave her the (self-) permission to reflect on what she most believed in. It was made clear to her, when she arrived in the United States, that she could remove her hijab and, if she pursued this, no one from her home community would even know that she had chosen to be uncovered. She considered this option. But she realized she wanted to continue to cover not for anyone in her native country but for herself. She reconciled earlier messages about Islam that she saw as contradictory. For example, she learned that one can still be a faithful Muslim if a woman does not cover or does not know Arabic, and she resolved that it was worse to wear a hijab if someone did not want to or did not believe in the purpose of covering.
Near the end of the interview, another woman was asked if she believed it was important to question religion. She responded:
Oh yes, definitely. But I also believe that questioning [pause] it stops somewhere. It’s all about your experiences, your lifestyles, how you’re growing up, which country you’re coming from. Religion comes from these resources, all together. But yeah, I questioned it. It’s the best religion to me which fits my life and my ideology.
This informant, as with others, perceived that the process of questioning aspects of her prior understanding of how culture and religion were intertwined eventually led her to adopt an ideology to which she would extract meaning and could be faithful. The challenge for some participants reflected the privileges of living in a society where freedoms were seen as a birthright but the presence of living an affirming faith are found in countries where freedoms are not seen in the same way. One phrased it this way:
The thing is, like, I like the freedom here. But at the same time, I want to move back to, you know, an Arab country. Because I like the Deen and hearing the prayer, athan, every day, and just being close to God, you know? Because, when you’re here, you just – time flies. You just lose track of time. You’re busy with work, homework, watching TV. But over there, time is more like going by really slow and you’re reminded of, you know, your God and you just, you know, feel like enjoying yourself. And getting close to God again.
This experience reflected to duality we heard from participants. Although they understood living in the United States offered them many luxuries (i.e., from living in sanitary conditions to living in a largely peaceful cultural climate where persons do not fear the random detonation of explosives), the process of reviewing, questioning, and responding to intellectual and affective challenges regarding their spiritual and religious senses of self extended and elaborated a fidelity to their individual affiliations with faith.
Qualitative-quantitative mixed designs provide an opportunity for different forms of data to be combined or merged. We reported that the women in our sample reported high scores on the Reflective Commitment subscale of the Islamic Reflections Scale (Dover et al., 2007). We developed a numerical coding scheme that was applied to the portions of narrative text regarding the extent to which participants engaged in the pursuit of questioning and reflecting upon challenges as they incorporated new information and interpretation into their personalized commitment to Islam. This process allowed us to quantify qualitative data. We trained someone not associated with the study to code narrative text and to assign a numerical value (based on the coding scheme) to text data. Even with a small sample, the degree to which participants reported the importance of questioning and challenging oneself and Reflective Commitment was positively associated, r = .72, p < .005. Thus, there was agreement between responses on a self-reported scale measuring Reflective Commitment and the extent to which the independently coded narratives extolled the degree to which they questioned, brought resolution to, and incorporated a personally rich concept of faith.
Discussion
This study, using purposive sampling, explored the experiences of Muslim women who had moved to the United States. Although women immigrated from a variety of countries, a common feature across their home countries was that Islam is a dominant religion. Thus, the women in our study moved from a country where their religion is prominent to a country where the same religion is far less common and is often misunderstood or politically maligned by cultural sources of power. About two-thirds of the women in our sample were married. Single women migrated to the United States to matriculate into college, or graduate or professional training programs. Among the married women, some accompanied husbands as they began university programs, and in other couples both husbands and wives matriculated into the university. Participants knew of media-promulgated discourses of caution blanketed to minorities traveling to the U.S. That is, although women in our study did not report that they experienced sustained negative treatment which they believed stemmed from Islamophobia, that is not to say the women were uninformed about the ways in which Islamophobia works outside of their home countries. Some women, for example, were cautioned by family about traveling to a country where sentiments against Islam are prevalent (i.e., institutional or social approval for Islamophobia); and some women reported hearing about, witnessing, or experiencing differential treatment in airports which they attributed to others’ designation of them as Muslim. But, on the whole, women still looked forward to the opportunities for themselves, their families, and the professional training that they would find in the U.S.
Although formalized hypothesis testing, at least those as understood within positivistic epistemologies, usually are not used in qualitatively based studies, this study relied on basic tenets of the classic contact hypothesis. We suspected that women, living in their native countries, would be keenly aware of the cultural and political tropes dispersed by powerful media complexes in U.S. about and against both Islam as an Abrahamic religion and individual Muslims. But if so, once arriving in a new location where they knew no one, women (or women and their husbands) would be tasked with developing new relationships: with neighbors, with other students or colleagues, and with persons with whom they might share fellowship. At the most basic level, the contact hypothesis posits intergroup relations can be improved when persons from disparate histories interact with one another.
During their interviews, all informants recognized that the opportunity to move to the United States – either as they or their spouses pursued higher education – privileged their futures in ways that likely would not have been possible otherwise. Consistent with the contact hypothesis, we found that women perceived moving to a small city (where very few other Muslims lived) propelled women and their husbands to meet (non-Muslim) Americans and to interact with them. Participants reported perceiving that, as they got to know their neighbors and university peers/coworkers, Americans were genuinely interested in learning about their histories and generous in including them into social groups. As they became more involved and invested in these relationships, respondents recognized that they benefitted from a more expansive social community. For example, participants whose children interacted with other children (e.g., at preschool) valued seeing children interact and play together without religious- or ethnic-based prejudices. At least for the women in our study – those who might be referred to as the outgroup in terms of ingroup-outgroup relations – social contact was seen as destabilizing and ultimately minimizing the preconceived assumptions participants reported they once believed about how Americans would feel about and treat them. Although there were some who assumed they would eventually return to home country or a country where Islam is a primary religion, all participants perceived that their residency in the U.S. had given them a promising opportunity to see themselves in broader and more inclusive ways.
Because they trusted the bidirectional relationships they built with others, participants narrated ways in which they hoped they could reciprocate kindness and courtesy in the sharing of their cultural histories. Once living in the U.S., a few women reported having heard of negative comments directed at other Muslims. But no respondents perceived that they were actively mistreated because of their religion and, to the contrary, the far majority of women reported perceiving their American neighbors and friends were interested in learning from them about their histories: familial, religious, and cultural. Only one woman in our sample reported an experience that she saw as discriminatory, but she stated repeatedly she thought this was attributable to her race, not her religion. Participants hoped that, through new relationships with Americans, they could serve, inadvertently, as ambassadors of how Muslims live values of peace and concern for their community. As one informant said, “It is possible to change their mind [stereotypes about Islam and practicing Muslims]. Because, for example, when they in their daily lives live with us, they see that we are peaceful.”
In addition to the relationships that women built and maintained with Americans, they also forged relationships with other Muslims in their new community. To these women, meeting and interacting with Muslims who themselves were from other countries was perceived as a transformative opportunity. It was through these relationships that most participants interacted firsthand with other Muslims not from their home country; as a result, this was the first time they learned from others how Islam is anchored and practiced in other countries. It was from these relationships that participants learned what may have been presented to them as a religious doctrine was, rather, a cultural tradition. Interacting with Muslims from other countries – countries with various traditions, potentially disparate governmental structures – expanded their schemes for additional ways in which they could conceptualize and enact Islam into their lives. Opportunities to hear and learn about other customs, to reflect on the historical legacies of customs from one’s home country, and (for some) dedicated academic course-based learning, positioned participants to dedicate time and reflection into solidifying how each wanted to incorporate existing and new practices of faith in her life. They perceived their experiences of moving to the U.S., developing relationships with (non-Muslim) Americans and Muslims from other countries – as they interacted with one another – as conduits to expand themselves intrapersonally and in terms of their faith. The women in the current study perceived that the confluence of several life events led them to reflect on their faith and religious orthodoxy in ways that they had not expected. Participants reported learning about Islam in ways that they had not learned in their home countries, and in challenging themselves to be open to the interpretations of others – including accepting that others’ customs and ideologies were valid – ultimately grounded their faith in new and more expansive ways.
There are strengths and limitations associated with all studies. The current study used a mixed design to capture a fuller experience in our sample than either a qualitative or quantitative method may have done alone. In relation to core questions we explored through biographic interviews, we reached data saturation (Corbin & Strauss, 2008; Hennink et al., 2015) but we had hoped for a sample of 15-16. Although 13 women participated in the study, interpretive analyses were interrogated on the narratives of 12 women. The audio files from one participant, even with two high-end microphones, were so quiet that a transcript could not be created from that interview. Had we been able to include even a few more participants, it may have been possible to perform a constant comparative analysis of women from the same region or country. In addition, the participants in this study could all both write and speak English; some had taken years of English as part of formal schooling in their home countries. Thus, it is possible the sample participants may be different in some ways than women who are less proficient in English or may have felt inhibited participating in the study. On the other hand, we have no evidence that there was any systematic reason for women to have opted out of participation. In this study, Muslim women reported positive experiences living in a new country. During interviews, a majority of participants reported that they knew of the negative treatment some immigrants and some Muslim immigrants had received once having moved to the United States, but perhaps a more compelling result from the women in our study was that they reported very few negative experiences, none including anything they identified as severe. On the other hand, a different sample of participants, including women or men, who felt they had experienced effects of Islamophobia, may have yielded a different perspective on living in a new country.
We recognize the sample size does not lend itself to inferential statistical procedures, especially if this were a quantitatively based study with a goal of testing differences between groups; however, we believe that the descriptive data provided from established scales provides an important addition in characterizing our sample’s attitudes and belief systems. The biographic data shared by the women in this study provided contextually rich data, narrative data that many large-scale studies using only quantitative measures would be less likely to interrogate. We make no claim the narratives reported by these women, nor our interpretive analyses, characterize all or even most Muslim women. Still, it is possible that core outcomes of our interpretive data (e.g., how new relationships facilitate perceptions of wellbeing and social connectedness) could be useful to university or employment personnel as they work to maximize international students’ transitions to a new community.
Concluding Thoughts
Allport’s theoretically rich contact hypothesis suggests individuals or groups with different, often orthogonal, social groupings can learn to minimize their differences if they have positive contact with one another (1954). In the current study, Muslim women who had recently moved to the United States completed a series of established scales and participated in a biographic interview. Using reliable measures, we found that women in this sample scored similarly to the population on measures of psychosocial adjustment, scored high on various measures of spirituality, and reported that, on the whole, participants did not perceive that they had experienced discrimination or poor treatment because of their religion. Interpretive analyses based on individual interviews complemented the contact hypothesis in a number of ways. First, participants reported that they perceived warm, welcoming, and kind-hearted treatment from the persons they met in their new community; this was, in fact, contrary to many messages they had received in their home countries prior to departing those countries en route to the U.S. Second, participants came to learn that many countries – the United States as well as their home country – propagandize sweeping narratives against others that are sometimes, if not often, untrue. The women in this study, almost without exception, perceived the contact with Americans as not only contradictory to the global messaging about American immigration and opinions about Muslims, but was far more in-line with earlier, more savory, tropes about American generosity. And finally, participants’ contact with Muslims who they met in America – importantly, Muslims from countries with varying cultural traditions as related to Islam – served to expand and enrich women’s personal engagement with spirituality.
References
- Abu-Raiya, H., Pargament, K. I., & Mahoney, A. (2011). Examining coping methods with stressful interpersonal events experienced by Muslims living in the United States following the 9/11 attacks. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 3(1), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020034
- Abu-Ras, W. M., & Suárez, Z. E. (2009). Muslim men and women’s perception of discrimination, hate crimes, and PTSD symptoms post 9/11. Traumatology, 15(3), 48-63. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534765609342281
- Adam, Z., & Ward, C. (2016). Stress, religious coping and wellbeing in acculturating Muslims. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 10(2), 3-26. https://doi.org/10.3998/jmmh.10381607.0010.201
- Ahmed, S., & Ezzeddine, M. (2009). Challenges and opportunities facing American Muslim youth. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 4, 159-174. https://doi.org/10.1080/15564900903245782
- Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
- Amer, M. M., & Bagasra, A. (2013). Psychological research with Muslim Americans in the age of Islamophobia: Trends, challenges, and recommendations. American Psychologist, 68(3), 134-144. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032167
- Amir, Y. (1969). Contact hypothesis in ethnic relations. Psychological Bulletin, 71(5), 319-342. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027352
- Amri, S., & Bemak, F. (2013). Mental health help-seeking behaviors of Muslim immigrants in the United States: Overcoming social stigma and cultural mistrust. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 7(1), 43-63. https://doi.org/10.3998/jmmh.10381607.0007.104
- Andersson, G. (1996). The benefits from optimism: A meta-analytic review of The Life Orientation Test. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 719-725. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(96)00118-3
- Blackwood, L., Hopkins, N., & Reicher, S. D. (2015). ‘Flying while Muslim’: Citizenship and misrecognition in the airport. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 3(2), 148-170. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v3i2.375
- Bleich, E. (2011). How is Islamophobia and how much is there? Theorizing and measuring an emerging comparative concept. American Behavioral Scientist, 55(12), 1581-1600. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764211409387
- Ben-Ner, A., McCall, B. P., Stephane, M., & Wang, H. (2009). Identity and in-group/out-group differentiation in work and giving behaviors: Experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72(1), 153-170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2009.05.007
- Cahill, C. (2010). ‘Why do they hate us?’: Reframing immigration through participatory action research. Area, 42(2), 152-161. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-4762.2009.00929.x
- Carroll, L. (2015, November 22). Fact-checking Trump’s claim that thousands in New Jersey cheered when the World Trade Center tumbled. [Web blog post]. Retrieved from: www.politifact.com/truth-o-meter/statements/2015/nov/22/donald-trump-/fact-checking-trumps-claim-thousands-new-jersey-ch/
- Charmaz, K. (2014). Constructing grounded theory. (2nd ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
- Ciftci, A., Jones, N., & Corrigan, P. W. (2013). Mental health stigma in the Muslim community. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 7(1), 17-32. https://doi.org/10.3998/jmmh.10381607.0007.102
- Cimino, R. (2005). ‘No God in common’: American evangelical discourse on Islam after 9/11. Review of Religious Research, 47(2), 162-174. https://doi.org/10.2307/3512048
- Chacón, J. M. (2017). Immigration and the Bully Pulpit. Harvard Law Review Forum, 130(7), 243-268.
- Choma, B. L., Haji, R., Hodson, G., & Hoffarth, M. (2016). Avoiding cultural contamination: Intergroup disgust sensitivity and religious identification as predictors of interfaith threat, faith-based policies, and Islamophobia. Personality and Individual Differences, 95, 50-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.013
- Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research (3rd ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
- Côté, R. R., & Erickson, B. H. (2009). Untangling the roots of tolerance: How forms of social capital shape attitudes toward ethnic minorities and immigrants. American Behavioral Scientist, 52(12), 1664-1689. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764209331532
- Creswell, J. W. (2014). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods approaches (4th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
- Crisp, R. J., & Turner, R. N. (2009). Can imagined interactions produce positive perceptions: Reducing prejudice through simulated contact. American Psychologist, 64(4), 231-240. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014718
- Davids, N. (2014). Muslim women and the politics of religious identity in a (post) secular society. Studies in Philosophy and Education, 33, 303-313. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11217-013-9389-9
- DeAngelis, T. (2001, November). All you need is contact. APA Monitor, 32(10). Retrieved from: https://www.apa.org/monitor/nov01/contact.aspxhttps://doi.org/10.1037/e312492004-026
- Dover, H., Miner, M., & Dowson, M. (2007). The nature and structure of Muslim religious reflection. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 2, 189-210. https://doi.org/10.1080/15564900701614858
- Dixon, J., Durrheim, K., & Tredoux, C. (2005). Beyond the optimal contact strategy: A reality check for the contact hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(7), 697-711. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.7.697
- Dixon, J., Tropp, L. R., Durrheim, K., & Tredoux, C. (2010). “Let them eat harmony”: Prejudice-reduction strategies and attitudes of historically disadvantaged groups. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(2), 76-80. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721410363366
- Ensari, N., & Miller, N. (2002). The out-group must not be so bad after all: The effects of disclosure, typicality, and salience on intergroup bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 313-329. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.313
- Everett, J. A. C., Schellhaas, F. M. H., Earp, B. D., Ando, V., Memarzia, J., Parise, C. V., Fell, B., & Hewstone, M. (2015). Covered in stigma? The impact of differing levels of Islamic head-covering on explicit and implicit biases toward Muslim women. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 45, 90-104. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12278
- Fisher, P., Ai, A. L., Aydin, N., Frey, D., & Haslam, S. A. (2010). The relationship between religious identity and preferred coping strategies: An examination of the relative importance of interpersonal and intrapersonal coping in Muslim and Christian faiths. Review of General Psychology, 14(4), 365-381. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021624
- Franklin, J. D., Smith, W. A., & Hung, M. (2014). Racial battle fatigue for Latina/o students: A quantitative perspective. Journal of Hispanic Higher Education, 13(4), 303-322. https://doi.org/10.1177/1538192714540530
- Furseth, I. (2011). The Hijab: Boundary work and identity negotiations among immigrant Muslim women in the Los Angeles area. Review of Religious Research, 52(4), 365-385.
- Gaddis, S. M., & Ghoshal, R. (2015). Arab American housing discrimination, ethnic competition, and the contact hypothesis. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 660, 282-299. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716215580095
- Gaertner, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., & Bachman, B. A. (1996). Revisiting the contact hypothesis: The induction of a common ingroup identity. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 20(3/4), 271-290. https://doi.org/10.1016/0147-1767(96)00019-3
- Ghaffari, A., & Ciftci, A. (2010). Religiosity and self-esteem of Muslim immigrants to the UnitedStates: The moderating role of perceived discrimination. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 20, 14-25. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508610903418038
- Ghumman, S., & Ryan, A. (2013). Not welcome here: Discrimination towards women who wear the Muslim headscarf. Human Relations, 66, 671-698. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726712469540
- Giroux, H. A. (2017). White nationalism, armed culture and state violence in the age of Donald Trump. Philosophy & Social Criticism, 43(9), 887-910. https://doi.org/10.1177/0191453717702800
- Goforth, A. N., Oka, E. R., Leong, F. T. L., & Denis, D. J. (2014). Acculturation, acculturative stress, religiosity and psychological adjustment among Muslim Arab American adolescents. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 8(2), 3-19. https://doi.org/10.3998/jmmh.10381607.0008.202
- Goldfried, J., & Miner, M. (2002). Quest religion and the problem of limited compassion. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41(4), 685-695. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-5906.00154
- Halliday, F. (1999). Islamophobia reconsidered. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 22, 892-902. https://doi.org/10.1080/014198799329305
- Hartigan, E. A. (2006/2007). Engaged surrender in the void: Post-secularist “human” rights discourse and Muslim feminists. Journal of Law and Religion, 22(1), 131-151. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0748081400003234
- Hassouneh, D. M., & Kulwicki, A. (2007). Mental health, discrimination, and trauma in Arab Muslim women living in the US: A pilot study. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 10(3), 257-262. https://doi.org/10.1080/13694670600630556
- Hjelmgaard, K. (2018, November 16). What you don’t know about America’s Islamic heritage. USA Today. Retrieved from: www.usatoday.com/story/news/world/2018/11/16/islam-america-muslims-religion-president-trump-american/173687002/https://doi.org/10.1097/01.NME.0000529947.61216.b3
- Hauslohner, A. (2017, July 26). Discrimination among Muslims is increasing in U.S., Pew study finds. The Washington Post. Retrieved from: www.washingtonpost.com/national/discrimination-against-muslims-is-increasing-in-us-pew-study-finds/2017/07/25/dfa52756-717a-11e7-9eac-d56bd5568db8_story.html
- Hennink, M., Hutter, I., & Bailey, A. (2015). Qualitative research methods. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
- Henry, P. J., & Hardin, C. D. (2006). The contact hypothesis revisited: Status bias in the reduction of implicit prejudice in the United States and Lebanon. Psychological Science, 17(10), 862-868. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01795.x
- Holland, J. C., Kash, K. M., Passik, S., Gronert, M. K., Sison, A., Lederberg, M., Russak, S. M., Baider, L., & Fox, B. (1998). A brief Spiritual Beliefs Inventory for use in quality of life research in life-threatening illness. Psycho-Oncology, 7, 460-469. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1611(199811/12)7:6<460::AID-PON328>3.0.CO;2-R
- Hu, C., Pazaki, H., Al-Qubbaj, K., & Cutler, M. (2009). Gender identity and religious practices of first-generation Muslim women immigrants in the U.S. Making Connections: Interdisciplinary approaches to cultural diversity, 11(1), 50-63.
- Huber, L. P. (2016). “Make America Great Again!”: Donald Trump, racist nativism, and the virulent adherence to white supremacy amid U.S. demographic change. Charleston Law Review, 10, 215-248.
- Hutchison, P., & Rosenthal, H. E. S. (2011). Prejudice against Muslims: Anxiety as a mediator between intergroup contact and attitudes, perceived group variability and behavioural intentions. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 34(1), 40-61. https://doi.org/10.1080/01419871003763312
- Islam, M., & Hewstone, M. (1993). Intergroup attributions and affective consequences in majority and minority groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(6), 936-950. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.936
- Jasperse, M., Ward, C., & Jose, P. E. (2012). Identity, perceived religious discrimination, and psychological well-being in Muslim immigrant women. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 61(2), 250-271. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2011.00467.x
- Jung, J. H. (2012). Islamophobia? Religion, contact with Muslims, and respect for Islam. Review of Religious Research, 54, 113-126. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13644-011-0033-2
- Khan, M., & Ecklund, K. (2012). Attitudes toward Muslim Americans post 9/11. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 7(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.3998/jmmh.10381607.0007.101
- Khan, Z. H., Watson, P. J., & Habib, F. (2005). Muslim attitudes toward religion, religious orientation, and empathy among Pakistanis. Mental Health, Religion, and Culture, 8(1), 49-61. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674670410001666606
- Kunst, J. R., Sadeghi, T., Tahir, H., Sam, D., & Thomsen, L. (2016). The vicious circle of religious prejudice: Islamophobia makes the acculturation attitudes of majority and minority members clash. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 249-259. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2174
- Lajevardi, N., & Oskooii, K. A. R. (2018). Old-fashioned racism, contemporary Islamophobia, and the isolation of Muslim Americans in the age of Trump. Journal of Race, Ethnicity, and Politics, 3, 112-152. https://doi.org/10.1017/rep.2017.37
- Lopes, B., & Jaspal, R. (2015). Paranoia predicts out-group prejudice: Preliminary experimental data. Mental Health, Religion, & Culture, 18(5), 380-395. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2015.1065475
- Major, B., Blodorn, A., & Blascovich, G. M. (2018). The threat of increasing diversity: Why many White Americans support Trump in the 2016 presidential election. Group Processes &Intergroup Relations, 21(6), 931-940. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430216677304
- Marranci, G. (2004). Multiculturalism, Islam, and the clash of civilisations theory: Rethinking Islamophobia. Culture and Religion, 5(1), 105-117. https://doi.org/10.1080/0143830042000200373
- Martin, M. B. (2015). Perceived discrimination of Muslims in health care. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 9(2), 41-69. https://doi.org/10.3998/jmmh.10381607.0009.203
- Merino, S. M. (2010). Religious diversity in a “Christian Nation”: The effects of theological exclusivity and interreligious contact on the acceptance of religious diversity. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 49(2), 231-246. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5906.2010.01506.x
- Mir, S. (2011). ‘Just to make sure people know I was born here’: Muslim women constructing American selves. Discourse: Studies in the Cultural Politics of Education, 32(4), 547-563. https://doi.org/10.1080/01596306.2011.601552
- Mishra, S., & Shirazi, F. (2010). Hybrid identities: American Muslim women speak. Gender, Place and Culture, 17(2), 191-209. https://doi.org/10.1080/09663691003600306
- Moffic, H. S., Peteet, J., Zakaria Hankir, A., & Awaad, R. (2019). Islamophobia and psychiatry: Recognition, prevention, and treatment. Cham, Switzerland: Springer Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-00512-2
- Mohamed, B. (2018, January 3). New estimates show U.S. Muslim population continues to grow. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from: www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/01/03/new-estimates-show-u-s-muslim-population-continues-to-grow/
- Moradi, M., & Hasan, N. T. (2004). Arab American persons’ reported experiences of discrimination and mental health: The mediating role of personal control. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 418-428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.51.4.418
- Mouhktar, S. F., & Walters, A. S. (2015). The mirror has two faces: Muslim-identified women’s perceptions about veiling. In A. D. Warner (Ed.), Ethnic and cultural identity: Perceptions, discrimination, and social challenges (pp. 145-167). New York, NY: Nova Publishers.
- Nadal, K. L., Griffin, K. E., Hamit, S., Leon, J., Tobio, M., & Rivera, D. P. (2012). Subtle and overt forms of Islamophobia: Microaggressions toward Muslim Americans. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 6(2), 15-37. https://doi.org/10.3998/jmmh.10381607.0006.203
- Nuzzi, O. (2017, September 11). Trump still hasn’t retracted his false claim that he saw Muslims cheering on 9/11. [Web blog post]. Retrieved from: nymag.com/intelligencer/2017/09/trump-hasnt-retracted-claim-about-muslims-cheering-9-11.html
- O’Connor, A. J., & Jahan, F. (2014). Under surveillance and overwrought: American Muslims’emotional and behavioral responses to government surveillance. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 8(1), 95-106. https://doi.org/10.3998/jmmh.10381607.0008.106
- Oveis, C., Horberg, E. J., & Keltner, D. (2010). Compassion, pride, and social intuitions of self-other similarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(4), 618-630. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017628
- Plante, T. G., & Boccaccini, M. T. (1997). The Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith Questionnaire. Pastoral Psychology, 45(5), 375-387. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02230993
- Pettigrew, T. F. (2009). Secondary transfer effect of contact: Do intergroup contact effects spread to noncontacted outgroups? Social Psychology, 40, 55-65. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335.40.2.55
- Pettigrew, T. F., Christ, O., Wagner, U., & Stellmacher, J. (2007). Direct and indirect intergroup contact effects on prejudice: A normative interpretation. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 31, 411-425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2006.11.003
- Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2005). Hypothesis: It’s history and influence. In J. F. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L. A. Rudman (Eds.), On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport (pp. 262-277). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
- Pettigrew, T. F. (2017). Social psychological perspectives on Trump supporters. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 5(1), 107-116. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v5i1.750
- Pew Research Center. (2017, July 26). U.S. Muslims concerned about their place in society, but continue to believe in the American dream. Retrieved from: www.pewforum.org/2017/07/26/findings-from-pew-research-centers-2017-survey-of-us-muslims/
- Phillips, D., & Lauterbach, D. (2017). American Muslim immigrant mental health: The role of racism and mental health stigma. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 11(1), 39-56. https://doi.org/10.3998/jmmh.10381607.0011.103
- Pirani, F. (2018, June 13). Nearly 1-in-5 Americans would deny Muslim American citizens the right to vote, new report finds. The Atlanta Journal-Constitution. Retrieved from: https://www.ajc.com/news/national/nearly-americans-would-deny-muslim-american-citizens-the-right-vote-new-report-finds/3JaLmKciEigUGNSjG27QWN/
- Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306
- Rippy, A. E., & Newman, E. (2008). Adaptation of a scale of race-related stress for use with Muslim Americans. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 3(1), 53-68. https://doi.org/10.1080/15564900802035292
- Rowatt, W. C., Franklin, L. M., & Cotton, M. (2005). Patterns and personality correlates of implicit and explicit attitudes toward Christians and Muslims. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 44(1), 29-43. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5906.2005.00263.x
- Ruggiero, K. M., & Taylor, D. M. (1997). Why minority group members perceive or do not perceive the discrimination that confronts them: The role of self-esteem and perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(2), 373-389. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.373
- Salem, J. M. (2013). Citizenship and religious expression in the West: A comparative analysis of experiences of Muslims in France, Germany, and the USA. Journal of Muslim Minority Affairs, 33(1), 77-92. https://doi.org/10.1080/13602004.2013.791189
- Samari, G. (2016). Islamophobia and public health in the United States. American Journal of Public Health, 106, 1920-1925. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2016.303374
- Samari, G., Alcalá, H., & Sharif, M. Z. (2018). Islamophobia, health, and public health: A systematic literature review. American Journal of Public Health, 108(6), e1-e9. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2018.304402
- Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome experiences. Health Psychology, 4, 219-247. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.4.3.219
- Shammas, D. (2009). Post-9/11 Arab and Muslim American community college students: Ethno-religious enclaves and perceived discrimination. Community College Journal of Research and Practice, 33(3-4), 283-308. https://doi.org/10.1080/10668920802580507
- Sheridan, L. P. (2006). Islamophobia pre- and post-September 11th, 2001. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 317-336. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260505282885
- Sheridan, L. P., & Gillett, R. (2005). Major world events and discrimination. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 8, 191-197. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-839x.2005.00166.x
- Sides, J., & Gross, K. (2013). Stereotypes of Muslims and support for the war on terror. The Journal of Politics, 75(3), 583-598. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022381613000388
- Sides, J., Tesler, M., & Vavreck, L. (2017). The 2016 U.S. election: How Trump lost and won. Journal of Democracy, 28(2), 34-44. https://doi.org/10.1353/jod.2017.0022
- Smith, W. A., Mustaffa, J. B., Jones, C. M., Curry, T. J., & Allen, W. R. (201). ‘You make me wanna holler and throw up both my hands!’: Campus culture, Black misandric microaggressions, and racial battle fatigue. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 29(9), 1189-1209. https://doi.org/10.1080/09518398.2016.1214296
- Smith, W. A., Yosso, T. J., Solórzano, D. G. (2006). Challenging racial battle fatigue on historically white campuses: A critical race examination of race-related stress. In C. A. Stanley (Ed.), Faculty of color: Teaching in predominately white college and universities (pp. 299-327). Bolton, MA: Anker Publishing.
- Strauss, A. (1994). Transformations of identity. In A. Strauss (Ed.), Creating sociological awareness: Collective images and symbolic representations (pp. 313-338). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Press.
- Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory procedures and techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
- Stuart, J. (2014). A qualitative analysis of Muslim young adults’ adaptation experiences in New Zealand. Journal of Muslim Mental Health, 8(2), 21-46. https://doi.org/10.3998/jmmh.10381607.0008.203
- Taras, R. (2012). ‘Islamophobia never stands still’: Race, religion, and culture. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 36(3), 417-433. https://doi.org/10.1080/01419870.2013.734388
- Waldman, P. (2018, September 11). Trump’s long history of lying about 9/11 and exploiting it for personal gain. The Washington Post. Retrieved from: www.washingtonpost.com/blogs/plum-line/wp/2018/09/11/trumps-long-history-of-lying-about-9-11-and-exploiting-it-for-personal-gain/?utm_term=.48e3e5d97ccb
- Widner, D., & Chicoine, S. (2011). It’s all in the name: Employment discrimination against Arab Americans. Sociological Forum, 26, 806-823. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1573-7861.2011.01285.x
- Williams, R. H., & Vashi, G. (2007). Hijab and American Muslim women: Creating the space for autonomous selves. Sociology of Religion, 68(3), 269-287. https://doi.org/10.1093/socrel/68.3.269
- Yilmaz, I. (2016). The nature of Islamophobia: Some key features. In D. Pratt & R. Woodlock (Eds.), Fear of Muslims?: International perspectives on Islamophobia (pp. 19-29). Cham, Switzerland: Springer Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-29698-2_2
- Yosso, T. J., Smith, W. A., Ceja, M., & Solórzano, D. G. (2009). Critical race theory, racial micro aggressions, and campus racial climate for Latina/o undergraduates. Harvard Educational Review, 79(4), 659-691. https://doi.org/10.17763/haer.79.4.m6867014157m707l
- Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. (1988). The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52(1), 30-41. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2
- Zimmerman, D. D. (2014). Young Muslim women in the United States: Identities at the intersection of group membership and multiple individualities. Social Identities, 20(4-5), 299-313. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504630.2014.997202
Footnotes
As of 2001, over 500 studies were based off of Allport’s classic theoretical model (DeAngelis, 2001).
All but one participant self-identified as a woman of color. This designation may be important in that the community in which the study occurred is largely white and the cost of living is proportionately high relative to the overall state. Demographic data show that 73% of the population is white, 12% Native American (which is 12 times the national average for similar communities), 2% black/African American, 2% Asian/Asian American, 7% other, and 4% categorized as more than 1 race. The median income is $58,000, the median rent $1138.00 and the 5-room home price $297.00. Geographical and demographic characteristics actuate new social contact with existing members of a community, facilitating new relationships in line with the contact hypothesis.
All interviews were conducted by the second author who, at the beginning of each in-person interview, disclosed to participants from where in the Middle East her family had migrated and that she self-identifies as Muslim.